What We Know About What Dogs Know
A talk with author David Grimm about our changing views of the history of dogs, how certain breeds have developed for certain tasks and the rapidly advancing scientific study of just how dogs think.
What We Know About What Dogs Know
A talk with author David Grimm about our changing views of the history of dogs, how certain breeds have developed for certain tasks and the rapidly advancing scientific study of just how dogs think.
Modern Farmer: What’s your take on the relative intelligence of a dog that has been bred for agriculture or to work a job? Are they smarter than a house pet?
David Grimm: Scientists don’t like using the word ‘intelligence’ anymore. The idea is that every animal evolved to be the best at what it is best at. In other words, we may think we are smarter than dogs, but if you gave us an olfactory test we would look really stupid compared to dogs. That is why you don’t hear the word ‘intelligence’ a lot. People focus on what the animal is best at, so it’s fair to say that some of the dogs that have been bred specially, like border collie and other dogs that have been bred for herding or hunting, are better than other breeds of dogs at those things. That cognition has been bred into them.
MF: Abilities are things you can breed over generational lines?
DG: Yes, definitely. I think that goes back to the very first dogs who evolved from wolves. We had animals that we at first didn’t really know what to do with. They seemed kind of useful: They were friendlier than wolves, potentially malleable, and I think in the early days – between 15,000 and 30,0000 years ago – the first things we were breeding into dogs was the ability to help us hunt, guard our campsite and, when livestock came on the scene, to protect and herd.
MF: How much do we know about the early days of how man and dog interacted?
DG: Nothing is actually known, but there are a couple competing theories. One is that early humans had found some wolf pups, raised them and over the generations bred them to be dogs. That theory has fallen out of favor. Dogs are the very first things we domesticated, so we would have had no idea we could take one animal and turn it into another. So that seems like a fantasy theory now, though there are still some scientists who espouse it.
We may think we are smarter than dogs, but if you gave us an olfactory test we would look really stupid compared to dogs.
Probably what happened is that dogs self-domesticated. The idea is that as early humans were traveling from campsite to campsite and living in villages we had these big trash piles and wolves started following us and lurking around the edges of villages. Over the generations the wolves would get closer and closer. Because they got closer, they got more food and survived better than the more scared wolves. So we have hundreds or thousands of years where the wolves are getting closer and closer and closer, until they are basically living among us and aren’t afraid of us.
What scientists are converging on is that dogs went through this first stage of domestication and once they were living among us, we started much more selectively breeding them and training them. That was a second phase of domestication, where dogs became more attuned to us. Cats never went through that second phase and are much more aloof and harder to train. Cats came on the scene about 10,000 years ago when we were very much farmers and we had all this grain and storage which brought mice and rats. This attracted cats.
MF: Do we have a sense of what people were doing with dogs before the agricultural revolution?
DG: Its unclear. I spoke to a woman for my book and her hypothesis is that the first dogs were not really like the dogs we know today. They were like Siberian Huskies on steroids. These were very big and muscular animals. She even proposes that we could have used them as mules, like strapping firewood to them, just helping us transport stuff. That would have been useful in the hunter-gatherer days because we were not living in villages, but we were moving from campsite to campsite. At that point dogs would have been useful as cargo carriers and maybe helping us hunt. When we started settling down, it became more useful for them to be guarding our villages and maybe helping us herd. Once we settling down in villages and start domesticating livestock then we need animals to help us herd and protect flocks. That is where you are going to see new skills coming in with dogs.
MF: What do you think are some of the cognitive abilities of dogs that people aren’t aware of?
DG: One of the really cool things is about 15 years ago there were two groups of scientists, one at Duke University and one at Budapest. The studies showed if you have bunch of cups on the floor, and one with a treat in it, if you point to the cup with the treat in dogs will almost always go to the cup we point at. It doesn’t seem too complicated, but chimpanzees can’t do that. There has been this explosion in dog research since then. There is nearly a dozen labs around the world that study the canine mind, and it goes back to that experiment.
It is kind of a social skill. Children don’t learn to follow someone pointing until they’re about one year old. When children learn to follow pointing, it opens up a wide range of things for them. That’s how we teach them. They use it to interact with us and learn about the world around them. It is an incredibly important skill for a species.
Scientists have said the reason why dogs can figure out pointing and chimpanzees can’t is because dogs are very tuned into our species. They have been living with us for longer than any other species has, for tens of thousands of years. So they really need to be tuned into our social gestures. They need to know that when we throw a ball we are trying to play with them, not hurt them. A chimpanzee may think we are trying to hurt them when we do that. When we ask a dog to shake our hand, that’s a gesture of friendship and cooperation. Try to get a chimpanzee to shake your hand, it might bite your fingers off. Even though a chimpanzee is in more ways cognitively advanced than dogs are, they just don’t have these social skills.
MF: How does a working dog view its master? Do we have a sense of this?
DG: I don’t think we really have a sense of this. It is a great question whether the dog that is a pet feels it has a different relationship with its owner than a dog that is a working animal. The way that owners treat working animals varies. I think there are some working dogs that are not seen as pets but seen as coworkers and maybe companions, but rarely seen as family members. Does the dog that sleeps in the owner’s bed know that is is kind of a family member in comparison to the dog who works in the field all day and sleeps in a shed? I don’t know the answer to that. My guess is that a dog probably knows there is a difference in that relationship.
MF: What is it like when you run experiments on dogs? Are they usually eager to take part? Is it something that they view a task they can complete and receive rewards for ?
DG: Yes, I think that’s the reason we know so much about how dogs think. Dogs, for the most part, are very eager to participate in these experiments. You bring them into a lab and they’re happy to be around anybody and do anything your want them to do for a treat. Its been very easy to study dogs and put them through a variety of tests. Researchers have even put them in MRI machines, which is a pretty harrowing experience – even for a human. There is lots of noise and it’s a tight space. Dogs can actually be trained to sit still now in MRI machines, which is allowing us to really physically probe what is going on in their brains, which is really difficult to do with any other animals.
MF: Last question: Are you a dog owner yourself?
DG: I’m not. I do like dogs, but I grew up with cats. I have two right now.
Follow us
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
Want to republish a Modern Farmer story?
We are happy for Modern Farmer stories to be shared, and encourage you to republish our articles for your audience. When doing so, we ask that you follow these guidelines:
Please credit us and our writers
For the author byline, please use “Author Name, Modern Farmer.” At the top of our stories, if on the web, please include this text and link: “This story was originally published by Modern Farmer.”
Please make sure to include a link back to either our home page or the article URL.
At the bottom of the story, please include the following text:
“Modern Farmer is a nonprofit initiative dedicated to raising awareness and catalyzing action at the intersection of food, agriculture, and society. Read more at <link>Modern Farmer</link>.”
Use our widget
We’d like to be able to track our stories, so we ask that if you republish our content, you do so using our widget (located on the left hand side of the article). The HTML code has a built-in tracker that tells us the data and domain where the story was published, as well as view counts.
Check the image requirements
It’s your responsibility to confirm you're licensed to republish images in our articles. Some images, such as those from commercial providers, don't allow their images to be republished without permission or payment. Copyright terms are generally listed in the image caption and attribution. You are welcome to omit our images or substitute with your own. Charts and interactive graphics follow the same rules.
Don’t change too much. Or, ask us first.
Articles must be republished in their entirety. It’s okay to change references to time (“today” to “yesterday”) or location (“Iowa City, IA” to “here”). But please keep everything else the same.
If you feel strongly that a more material edit needs to be made, get in touch with us at [email protected]. We’re happy to discuss it with the original author, but we must have prior approval for changes before publication.
Special cases
Extracts. You may run the first few lines or paragraphs of the article and then say: “Read the full article at Modern Farmer” with a link back to the original article.
Quotes. You may quote authors provided you include a link back to the article URL.
Translations. These require writer approval. To inquire about translation of a Modern Farmer article, contact us at [email protected]
Signed consent / copyright release forms. These are not required, provided you are following these guidelines.
Print. Articles can be republished in print under these same rules, with the exception that you do not need to include the links.
Tag us
When sharing the story on social media, please tag us using the following: - Twitter (@ModFarm) - Facebook (@ModernFarmerMedia) - Instagram (@modfarm)
Use our content respectfully
Modern Farmer is a nonprofit and as such we share our content for free and in good faith in order to reach new audiences. Respectfully,
No selling ads against our stories. It’s okay to put our stories on pages with ads.
Don’t republish our material wholesale, or automatically; you need to select stories to be republished individually.
You have no rights to sell, license, syndicate, or otherwise represent yourself as the authorized owner of our material to any third parties. This means that you cannot actively publish or submit our work for syndication to third party platforms or apps like Apple News or Google News. We understand that publishers cannot fully control when certain third parties automatically summarize or crawl content from publishers’ own sites.
Keep in touch
We want to hear from you if you love Modern Farmer content, have a collaboration idea, or anything else to share. As a nonprofit outlet, we work in service of our community and are always open to comments, feedback, and ideas. Contact us at [email protected].by Jake Swearingen, Modern Farmer
June 10, 2014
Modern Farmer Weekly
Solutions Hub
Innovations, ideas and inspiration. Actionable solutions for a resilient food system.
ExploreExplore other topics
Share With Us
We want to hear from Modern Farmer readers who have thoughtful commentary, actionable solutions, or helpful ideas to share.
SubmitNecessary cookies are absolutely essential for the website to function properly. This category only includes cookies that ensures basic functionalities and security features of the website. These cookies do not store any personal information.
Any cookies that may not be particularly necessary for the website to function and are used specifically to collect user personal data via analytics, ads, other embedded contents are termed as non-necessary cookies.